Motivation Science: Controversies and Insights
Mimi Bong
Visit to download the full and correct content document: https://ebookmass.com/product/motivation-science-controversies-and-insights-mimi-b ong/


Visit to download the full and correct content document: https://ebookmass.com/product/motivation-science-controversies-and-insights-mimi-b ong/
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and certain other countries.
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press 198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, United States of America.
© Oxford University Press 2023
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, by license, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above.
You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer.
CIP data is on file at the Library of Congress
ISBN 978–0–19–766235–9
eISBN 978–0–19–766237–3
DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780197662359.001.0001
Acknowledgments
AboutthebMRISymposiumonMotivation
Contributors
Introduction
SECTION I WHAT IS MOTIVATION?
1. What Is Motivation?
What Is Motivation, Where Does It Come from, and How Does It Work?
CarolS.Dweck,MatthewL.Dixon,andJamesJ.Gross
Energization and Direction Are Both Essential Parts of Motivation
AndrewJ.Elliot
What Is Motivation?
EdwinA.Locke
Motivation Processes and Outcomes
DaleH.Schunk
Motivation Is the Interaction Between Dispositions and Context
DeborahStipek
Motivation Is the State of Wanting Something . . . But Do We Want the Right Things?
KennonM.Sheldon
Wanting to Feel Effective in Our Goal Pursuits for Both Outcomes and Process
E.ToryHigginsandEmilyNakkawita
Pleasure, Utility, and Goals: Motivation as a Value-Based Decision-Making Process
Sung-ilKim
Jingle-Jangle Fallacies in Motivation Science: Toward a Definition of Core Motivation
ReinhardPekrun
Academic Self-Concept: A Central Motivational Construct
GeetanjaliBasarkodandHerbertW.Marsh
Motivation Resides Only in Our Language, Not in Our Mental Processes
KouMurayama
InsightsGainedfromControversy1
SECTION II WHAT ARE THE CURRENT CONTROVERSIES IN MOTIVATION SCIENCE?
2. Are Motivational Processes Universal Across Cultures and Contexts?
Does One Size Fit All? Cultural Perspectives on School Motivation
DennisM.McInerney†
Where Will Michelle Go to College? Culture and Context in the Study of Motivation
PaulA.Schutz
Can We Really Say that Motivational Processes Are Universal Across Cultures and Contexts?
BrianaP.Green,DeLeonL.Gray,ElanC.Hope,andJamaal S.Matthews
Vitamins for Psychological Growth: A Universal Foundation for Motivating Others
BartSoenensandMaartenVansteenkiste
Big-Fish-Little-Pond Effect: Universality of Psychological Comparison Processes
HerbertW.MarshandGeetanjaliBasarkod
InsightsGainedfromControversy2
3. Is There Such a Thing as “Good” Motivation and “Bad” Motivation?
Some Motivations Make Us Happier Than Others
KennonM.Sheldon
The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly of Motivation
NikosNtoumanis
Less Is Sometimes More: Differentiating “Mustivation” from “Wantivation”
MaartenVansteenkisteandBartSoenens
Do We Sometimes Surrender Our Good Motivation for Bad? Some Reflections on the Quality of Motivation
AllanWigfield
Good Versus Bad Motivation? Avoiding the Lure of False Dichotomies
PatriciaA.Alexander
InsightsGainedfromControversy3
4. Does Extrinsic Incentive (e.g., Rewards, Competition) Undermine Motivation?
Extrinsic Rewards Undermine Motivation in the Classroom . . . Sometimes
EricM.Anderman
Extrinsic Incentives/Rewards: Short-Term Fix That Can Undermine Long-Term Motivation
WendyS.Grolnick
Interest and Its Relation to Rewards, Reward Expectations, and Incentives
SuzanneE.HidiandK.AnnRenninger
Competition Can Enhance Motivation—But Typically Undermines It
JohnmarshallReeve
InsightsGainedfromControversy4
5. Can We Control Our Motivation?
The Unconscious Sources of Motivation and Goals
JohnA.BarghandPeterM.Gollwitzer
Two Routes to the Self-Regulation of Motivation and Goals
PeterM.GollwitzerandJohnA.Bargh
The Uneasy Relationship Between Conscious and NonConscious Motivation
TimUrdan
Controlling Your Own Motivation Is an Acquired Skill
ChristopherA.Wolters
A Key to Motivation Is Thinking and Acting Like You Can Change Things
ErikaA.Patall
Finding the Second Wind: Motivation Is Within Our Control
EllenL.Usher
InsightsGainedfromControversy5
6. Can You Distinguish Motivation from Cognition and Emotion?
Cognitions and Emotions Energize and Sustain Motivation
DaleH.Schunk
Dissecting the Elephant: Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation as Distinct but Intertwined Entities
ReinhardPekrun
Exploring the Boundaries Between Motivation, Cognition, and Emotion: Theoretical, Empirical, and Practical Distinctions
PatriciaA.Alexander
Transactions Among Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition: Blurring the Lines
PaulA.Schutz
Are Cognition, Motivation, and Emotion the Same or Different? Let’s Abandon That Thinking
KouMurayama
InsightsGainedfromControversy6
7. What Are the Unanswered Questions and Unresolved Controversies in Motivation Study?
Understanding Motivation: So Much Is Known, So Much Left to Learn
TimUrdan
How Does Context Shape Motivation?
MimiBong
Is a Focus on Looking Smart Beneficial for Students’ Engagement, Learning, and Achievement?
LisaLinnenbrink-Garcia
Is There a Need for Psychological Needs in Theories of Achievement Motivation?
AllanWigfieldandAlisonC.Koenka
Should Theoretical Integration Occur in the Motivation Literature? Considering What, for Whom, and When AlisonC.KoenkaandAllanWigfield
InsightsGainedfromControversy7
SECTION III HOW DO WE MOTIVATE PEOPLE?
8. How Do We Motivate People?
How Do We Motivate People? Connecting to People’s Existing Goals and Values
CarolS.Dweck
Creating a Motivating Learning Environment: Guiding Principles from Philosophy, Psychology, and Pedagogy
PatriciaA.Alexander
Easy to Get People to Do Things, More Challenging to Facilitate Their Motivation
WendyS.Grolnick
When It Comes to Motivating Others, What’s Easy Is Not Always What Works
ErikaA.Patall
Motivating People: It Depends on What, and It Depends on When
EricM.Anderman
How Do We Motivate People? By Working with Their SelfBeliefs
EllenL.Usher
A Control-Value Approach to Affective Growth
ReinhardPekrun
How to Foster Motivation? The Need-Based Motivating
Compass as a Source of Inspiration
MaartenVansteenkisteandBartSoenens
You Can Motivate Others by Nurturing Five Experiences
That Satisfy Their Need for Autonomy: Authentic Inner Compass, Authentic Intentions, and Freedom
AviAssor,MotiBenita,andYaelGeifman
Improving Social Contexts Can Enhance Student Motivation
KathrynR.Wentzel
What Teachers Need to Know About Promoting Student Motivation to Learn
HelenPatrick
InsightsGainedfromControversy8
9. What Is the Most Fundamental Limitation in Contemporary Motivation Theory and Research?
Is Academic Motivation a Tree Trunk, a Fan, a Wall, a Rope, a Snake, or a Spear? No, It’s an Elephant and It’s on Fire
EllenA.Skinner
Gaps in Contemporary Motivation Research: A Biopsychological Perspective
AndrewJ.MartinandEmmaC.Burns
Identifying the Role of Social Relationships in Motivating Students to Learn
KathrynR.Wentzel
Most Motivation Research in Education Is Not Yet Useful for Teachers
HelenPatrick
Motivational Researchers Must Move Beyond Linear Models to Consider Motivational Processes as Part of a Complex System
LisaLinnenbrink-Garcia
The Most Fundamental Limitation in Motivation Theory and Research Is Our Theories
EricM.Anderman
Infatuation with Constructs and Losing Sight of the Motivational Phenomenon
AviKaplan
Theoretical and Methodological Disintegration Is the Most Fundamental Limitation in Contemporary Motivation Research
BenjaminNagengastandUlrichTrautwein
InsightsGainedfromControversy9
10. What Will Be the Most Significant Development in Motivation Science in the Next Decade?
The Next Decade: Making Motivation the Foundation of Psychology Again
CarolS.Dweck
Harnessing Biopsychology and Mobile Technology to Develop Motivation Science in the Next Decade
AndrewJ.Martin,EmmaC.Burns,RogerKennett,andJoel Pearson
Digitization Will Bring Profound Changes in Educational Practice and Research on Motivation
UlrichTrautweinandBenjaminNagengast
Understanding Human Motivation and Action as a Complex Dynamic System
AviKaplan
Assessing Motivation Dynamically
DaleH.Schunk
Motivation in the Wild: Capturing the Complex Social Ecologies of Academic Motivation
EllenA.Skinner,ThomasA.Kindermann,JustinW.Vollet, andNicoletteP.Rickert
Community-Engaged Research: The Next Frontier in Motivation Science
DeLeonL.GrayandBrookeHarris-Thomas
InsightsGainedfromControversy10
References
NameIndex
SubjectIndex
The book would simply not exist without the 57 motivation scientists who contributed their essays, so our first expression of gratitude is to our authors. Another essential group who made the book possible was the editorial team at Oxford University Press—Abby Gross and Katharine Pratt. They shared our enthusiasm and vision for this volume from the very beginning and remained supportive throughout. We would also like to express our appreciation to Allan Wigfield at the University of Maryland, who provided excellent early critiques on the direction of the volume. The four anonymous reviewers of our book proposal offered many detailed and extremely helpful suggestions, and we thank them from the bottom of our hearts. We took their comments seriously and, by doing so, substantially improved the quality and scope of the volume. We shared this book-writing journey with the past and present members of the Brain and Motivation Research Institute (bMRI) at Korea University. The bMRI Symposium on Motivation has been possible because of their dedication and yearning for new learning. We hope they hear their voices in the pages of this volume. Finally, we would like to thank our families. We are eternally grateful for their love, encouragement, and support.
The bMRI Symposium on Motivation is an annual international symposium hosted by the Brain and Motivation Research Institute (bMRI) of Korea University in Seoul, Korea. Each year, leading figures in the field of motivation science visit Korea University to present their theories and findings. Below is a list of the international keynote speakers at the past bMRI Symposia:
Edward L. Deci, Judith Harackiewicz, Deborah Stipek, Allan Wigfield
Ruth Butler, Jacquelynne S. Eccles, Bernard Weiner, Kathryn R. Wentzel
Patricia A. Alexander, Suzanne E. Hidi, Reinhard Pekrun, Richard M. Ryan
Avi Assor, Wendy S. Grolnick, Kennon M. Sheldon, Maarten Vansteenkiste
Daniel Ansari, Layne Kalbfleisch
Andrew J. Martin, Allison M. Ryan
Tim Urdan, Ellen L. Usher
Erika A. Patall, Helen Patrick
Benjamin Nagengast, Christopher A. Wolters, Shirley L. Yu
Andrew J. Elliot, Herbert W. Marsh
K. Ann Renninger, Ulrich Trautwein (Symposium canceleddue to COVID-19) (Symposium heldonline due to COVID-19) John A. Bargh, Roy F. Baumeister
Eric M. Anderman, Lynley H. Anderman
The bMRI Symposium on Motivation began in 2009 and continues year after year. International and local scholars share and discuss their programs of research during this event.
Patricia A. Alexander, PhD
Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology
University of Maryland College Park, MD, USA
Eric M. Anderman, PhD
Department of Educational Studies
The Ohio State University
Columbus, OH, USA
Avi Assor, PhD
School of Education
Ben Gurion University of the Negev Beer Sheva, Israel
John A. Bargh, PhD
Department of Psychology
Yale University
New Haven, CT, USA
Geetanjali Basarkod, PhD
Institute for Positive Psychology and Education
Australian Catholic University
North Sydney, Australia
Moti Benita, PhD
School of Education
Ben Gurion University of the Negev Beer-Sheva, Israel
Mimi Bong, PhD
Department of Education
Korea University
Seoul, South Korea
Emma C. Burns, PhD School of Education
Macquarie University
Sydney, Australia
Matthew L. Dixon, PhD
Department of Psychology
Stanford University
Stanford, CA, USA
Carol S. Dweck, PhD
Department of Psychology
Stanford University
Stanford, CA, USA
Andrew J. Elliot, PhD
Department of Psychology
University of Rochester
Rochester, NY, USA
Yael Geifman, PhD
School of Education
Ben Gurion University of the Negev
Beer-Sheva, Israel
Peter M. Gollwitzer, PhD
Department of Psychology
New York University
New York, NY, USA
DeLeon L. Gray, PhD
Department of Teacher Education and Learning Sciences
North Carolina State University
Raleigh, NC, USA
Briana P. Green, MS
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Special Education
Michigan State University
East Lansing, MI, USA
Wendy S. Grolnick, PhD
Department of Psychology
Clark University
Worcester, MA, USA
James J. Gross, PhD
Department of Psychology
Stanford University
Stanford, CA, USA
Brooke Harris-Thomas, MA
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education
Michigan State University
East Lansing, MI, USA
Suzanne E. Hidi, PhD
Applied Psychology and Human Development
University of Toronto
Toronto, ON, Canada
E. Tory Higgins, PhD
Department of Psychology
Columbia University
New York, NY, USA
Elan C. Hope, PhD
Department of Psychology
North Carolina State University
Raleigh, NC, USA
Avi Kaplan, PhD
Department of Psychological Studies in Education
Temple University
Philadelphia, PA, USA
Roger Kennett, PhD, Bsc, DipEd
School of Education
University of New South Wales
Sydney, Australia
Sung-il Kim, PhD
Department of Education
Korea University
Seoul, South Korea
Thomas A. Kindermann, PhD
Department of Psychology
Portland State University
Portland, OR, USA
Alison C. Koenka, PhD
Department of Educational Psychology
University of Oklahoma
Norman, OK, USA
Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia, PhD
Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education
Michigan State University
East Lansing, MI, USA
Edwin A. Locke, PhD
Robert H. Smith School of Business
University of Maryland
College Park, MD, USA
Herbert W. Marsh, PhD, DSc
Institute for Positive Psychology and Education
Australian Catholic University
Sydney, Australia
Andrew J. Martin, PhD
School of Education
University of New South Wales
Sydney, Australia
Jamaal S. Matthews, PhD
Combined Program in Education and Psychology
University of Michigan
Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Dennis M. McInerney†, PhD
Department of Special Education and Counselling Education University of Hong Kong
Ting Kok, Hong Kong
Kou Murayama, PhD
Hector Research Institute of Education Sciences and Psychology
University of Tübingen
Tübingen, Germany
Benjamin Nagengast, Dr. phil.
Hector Research Institute of Education Sciences and Psychology
University of Tübingen
Tübingen, Germany
Emily Nakkawita, MA, MPhil
Department of Psychology
Columbia University
New York, NY, USA
Nikos Ntoumanis, PhD
Danish Centre of Motivation and Behaviour Science
Southern Denmark University
Odense, Denmark
Erika A. Patall, PhD
Rossier School of Education
University of Southern California
Los Angeles, CA, USA
Helen Patrick, PhD
Department of Educational Studies
Purdue University
West Lafayette, IN, USA
Joel Pearson, PhD School of Psychology
The University of New South Wales
Sydney, Australia
Reinhard Pekrun, PhD
Department of Psychology
University of Essex
Colchester, Essex, UK
Johnmarshall Reeve, PhD
Institute for Positive Psychology and Education
Sydney, Australia
K. Ann Renninger, PhD
Department of Educational Studies
Swarthmore College
Swarthmore, PA, USA
Nicolette P. Rickert, PhD
Department of Psychology
Georgia Southern University
Statesboro, GA, USA
Dale H. Schunk, PhD
Teacher Education and Higher Education
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
Greensboro, NC, USA
Paul A. Schutz, PhD
College of Education
University of Arizona
Tucson, AZ, USA
Kennon M. Sheldon, PhD
Department of Psychological Sciences
University of Missouri
Columbia, MO, USA
Ellen A. Skinner, PhD
Department of Psychology
Portland State University
Portland, OR, USA
Bart Soenens, PhD
Department of Developmental, Personality, and Social Psychology
Ghent University
Ghent, Belgium
Deborah Stipek, PhD
Graduate School of Education
Stanford University
Stanford, CA, USA
Ulrich Trautwein, Dr. Phil.
Hector Research Institute of Education Sciences and Psychology
University of Tübingen
Tübingen, Germany
Tim Urdan, PhD
Department of Psychology
Santa Clara University
Santa Clara, CA, USA
Ellen L. Usher
Office of Applied Scholarship and Education Science
Mayo Clinic
Rochester, MN, USA
Maarten Vansteenkiste, PhD
Department of Developmental, Social and Personality Psychology
Ghent University
Ghent, Belgium
Justin W. Vollet, PhD
Department of Psychology
The University of Texas Permian Basin
Odessa, TX, USA
Kathryn R. Wentzel, PhD
Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology
University of Maryland College Park, MD, USA
Allan Wigfield, PhD
Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology
University of Maryland College Park, MD, USA
Christopher A. Wolters, PhD Educational Studies
The Ohio State University Columbus, OH, USA
Motivation science has advanced tremendously in the past decade. However, it is now clear that future progress is going to be stalled by the extent of disagreement among motivation scientists to some basic, yet controversial, questions. To help move motivation science toward greater coherence, we decided to recruit prominent scholars to voice their contrasting perspectives. Such debate is not only interesting, but it also makes future research, discoveries, collaborations, and applications more fruitful. It is in this spirit that we put together a volume to address those controversies that are most likely to provide insight.
The three editors of this volume are or have been professors of educational psychology and faculty members of the Brain and Motivation Research Institute (bMRI) at Korea University. Since 2009, we have hosted an annual international symposium called the bMRI Symposium on Motivation, whose name we came up with to pay tribute to the pioneering Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (1953-present). Each year, we have invited leading figures in the field of motivation to deliver their keynote addresses (2009-present). In 2018, the bMRI Symposium on Motivation celebrated its 10th anniversary. Given this successful and well-received history, we saw the need for and interest in the present volume to commemorate the symposium’s 10-year history.
Because many excellent handbooks on motivation exist, we wanted to try something different—be provocative. We wanted to provoke creative ideas among the authors and readers. By sharing current
thinking and providing innovative insights into the important questions and controversies in the study of motivation, we wanted the volume to inform readers about cutting-edge theory and research in motivation that they can use to generate fresh and effective applications and interventions.
To achieve that end, we ask 10 thought-provoking questions that define contemporary motivation science’s most important, controversial, and provocative ideas. These questions deal with the nature of motivation, cultural differences in motivational processes, evidence-based strategies to enhance motivation, unresolved controversies, predictions of the future, and more. For each question, we invited multiple prominent scholars around the globe to provide their independent answers. This format allowed us to offer the reader multiple answers to each motivation question. No one right answer exists. Instead, multiple international authors share their views on each particular question. In this way, readers gain a rare opportunity to see how different theorists and researchers recognize, evaluate, and prescribe solutions to the same motivation problem. As editors, we hope the commonalities, disagreements, and uniqueness in the authors’ approaches will become evident to the readers.
This volume features four sections, each of which highlights a different core question:
•
•
•
•
What is motivation?
What are the current controversies in motivation science?
How do we motivate people?
What is the future of motivation science?
Within each section are more specific questions. We intentionally framed these more specific questions in a yes/no format to encourage the authors to agree or disagree with the controversial proposition and to explain the rationale behind their answers. Upon invitation, each author (or author team) chose up to three specific questions they wanted to address. As a result, a minimum of 4 to a maximum of 11 answers accompany each of the 10 specific questions in this volume.
Overall, the table of contents features 4 core questions, 10 specific and controversial questions, and 67 individual author responses. A total of 57 motivation scientists participated in answering one or more of the specific questions, allowing this volume to represent various unique and shared perspectives on each issue. Because the authors presented solid scientific evidence for their respective position based on their expertise and research record, it is not a right-or-wrong situation. Rather, we leave it to the readers to decide whose response or position they find most persuasive.
The volume targets the motivation science research community, graduate students, and a variety of professionals and practitioners (e.g., educators). While this represents our intended audience, we are optimistic that readers outside the field of contemporary motivation study will also find the book interesting, personally relevant, and professionally useful. To help make our volume attractive to a broad scope of readers, we asked the authors to tell some good stories in an informal and conversational tone and avoid jargon and technical terms as best they could. We wanted to make the book approachable for anyone interested in understanding motivational phenomena and improving the motivation of oneself and others, such as parents, employers/employees, and college students. To reach these more applied audiences, we added a concise discussion at the end of each specific question that
reiterates the main points articulated in the multiple essays and explains in plain language how the authors’ views are similar to or different from each other.
Putting this volume together has been a pleasant journey. The authors’ essays inspired our thinking and expanded our perspectives about the issues and controversies in contemporary motivation science. We are certain that readers will experience the same intellectual stimulation and satisfaction that we experienced while reading the authors’ insightful contributions. We have been deeply impressed by our motivation colleagues and their thinking about motivation. Without a single exception, all of the authors conveyed their passion and commitment to the study of motivation, which will be evident to the readers as they survey the essays. The greatest reward for us out of the whole process is our renewed respect for and confidence in both our colleagues and the current state of motivation science. It is our hope that this volume advances the 10 controversies and produces a whole new set of interesting questions for the next generation to consider.
In Seoul and Sydney, Mimi Bong, Johnmarshall Reeve, and Sung-il Kim
Motivation is often taken for granted in psychology, perhaps because it is hidden beneath the actions we take. The relative visibility of our actions and the relative invisibility of their motivational underpinnings may explain why some researchers—such as many who study cognition, intelligence, or personality—do not see their phenomena as arising from and importantly influenced by motivation. In fact, an eminent social-personality psychologist told one of us how his graduate school advisor had assured him that “motivation was assumed” and did not require further attention.
Our view is quite different. We see motivation as foundational to virtually all important actions or choices and therefore foundational for all forms of skilled performance or adaptive functioning. And, therefore, critical to acknowledge and understand.
But even for those of us who acknowledge and study motivation, many questions remain, and the purpose of this essay is to pose some of those questions and venture some answers to them.
Motivation can be defined as the processes that drive, select, and direct behaviors (see Reeve, 2018), but direct them toward what? Toward our goals. In simpler terms, motivation answers the
question: What do people want, and how do they go about getting it?
Where does motivation come from? How does it get started in the first place? To shed light on this, we turn to the concept of “needs.” We see needs as the crucible from which motivated behavior arises (see Deci & Ryan, 2000; Dweck, 2017, 2021; Reeve, 2018). We are all born with needs that jump-start the goal-oriented, motivated behaviors that are critical to survival and thriving. These are both physical needs (such as hunger and thirst) and psychological needs (such as the need for social relationships, optimal predictability, and competence).
In fact, we are built not only to experience these needs but also to fulfill them. For example, babies have built-in attentional and behavioral preparedness that helps them orient toward and interact with hunger-fulfilling, relationship-building, and competence-building situations. Babies also have truly remarkable cognitive, informationprocessing, and inferential abilities that are present at birth to help them learn about need-fulfilling situations and further ensure that these critical needs will be met in the future (Dweck, 2017, 2021). Thus, we propose that these needs form the basis upon which human motivation is built.
We further propose that needs come to life through specific goals. That is, needs and the opportunities to fulfill them are not enough. Fulfilling needs requires a specific goal (a mental representation—an “image” in the mind—of a desired state) and then goal-oriented behavior designed to bring one closer to that state. Indeed, we view goals as the central concept in motivation (see Fishbach & Ferguson, 2007).
How does motivation change with development and experience?
As children (and adults) pursue need-fulfilling goals in the context of their world (family, culture) and their biological make-up (e.g., temperament), they learn about what works and doesn’t work. That is, they learn which actions in which situations lead to pleasurable outcomes and which don’t. They then use this information to build models of themselves, other people, and their world and how it all works, and they use these models to guide future goal choices and
goal pursuit strategies (see Dweck, 2017, 2021). Of course, these models are flexible and take people well beyond the basic needs they are all born with.
As people develop, then, they build a repertoire of need-fulfilling goals and the means to achieve them. Over time, their pattern of goal choices and goal pursuit may become more recurrent, characteristic, or “trait-like” and may become what we might call their personality—their characteristic ways of thinking, feeling, and acting (see Dweck, 2021; McCabe & Fleeson, 2016). That is, we may see the developing patterns of goal pursuit as a basis for the development of personality.
How do people choose among the many goals they could pursue? Above, we defined goals as mental representations of desired states, but every moment presents us with many opportunities to imagine desired states and act in ways that bring us closer to those states. With all the internal stimuli (thoughts, feelings) and external stimuli bombarding us at any given time, how do we even begin to decide what we desire and what we wish to pursue?
Some theories propose that there is a dedicated brain area, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, that computes and compares the values of different possible goals and decides which is the best choice (e.g., Levy & Glimcher, 2012). Instead, we suggest that the winning goals are not chosen by an all-knowing overseer, but rather emerge from the pack based on the strength of their mental representation or, loosely speaking, the strength of their activation in our minds. Do we want to prepare for an upcoming presentation? Or FaceTime with a good friend? Or watch a video we’ve been eager to see? We suggest that both internal and external stimuli can heighten or dampen the representations of these candidate goals based on the costs and benefits of each; that is, how much we value that goal, how salient it is to us at that moment, and how easy or hard it will be to achieve.
Those goal representations that are activated more than others or activated beyond a certain threshold are the likely frontrunners to becoming active goals. We view this process as continuous and dynamic, so that goals can recede or come forward from one moment to the next. Yet it would not be adaptive to change course every other moment. Therefore, in this context, it is interesting to think about what sustains goals that require long-term pursuit so that they are not superseded or crowded out by momentary upstarts. Thus, we need to understand
•
•
•
how people invest longer-term goals with importance (greater representational strength) that lasts, how people create shorter-term, subordinate goals that are rewarding (in themselves and as markers of progress) to keep themselves on track toward the longer-term goals, and how other self-regulatory processes might help people to keep pursuing important longer-term goals even in the absence of more immediate rewards or obvious progress.
Are we saying that motivation is mostly about factors that are internal to the person? Many motivational processes, being psychological, can be considered internal, but contexts and environments are also critical. They not only shape people’s motivation over time and in the moment, but also are key to the success of people’s goal pursuits. In a recent study with a nationally representative sample, Yeager and his colleagues (2020, 2022; see also Yeager et al., 2019) examined the effects of a motivational intervention for US students making the difficult transition to high school. The intervention taught students a growth mindset: the idea that their intelligence is not fixed and that they can grow their brains and develop their academic and intellectual abilities. The program also included exercises in which students considered how they could put a growth mindset into practice in their schoolwork.
We found meaningful effects of the intervention on math grades for students whose math teachers held more of a growth mindset, but, interestingly, not for students whose math teachers held more of a fixed mindset. This was true even though students from both
contexts showed equal enthusiasm for the concept and equal desire to put it into action right after the intervention sessions. These findings suggest that teachers with more of a growth mindset created an environment in which students’ new or enhanced growth mindset beliefs and behaviors could be put into practice, take root, and turn into higher grades, whereas teachers with more of a fixed mindset did not. That is, when supportive practices or opportunities were lacking, enhanced motivation to pursue challenges and persist in the face of difficulty could not be sustained.
Is all behavior motivated? We propose that virtually all voluntary behavior is motivated. It is at some level chosen, initiated, pursued, and terminated (see, e.g., Kruglanski & Szumowska, 2020). It is at some level goal-directed no matter how trivial that goal may seem, such as shifting one’s position for a different proprioceptive or sensory experience.
However, not all motivated behavior is about overt actions. As we noted, motivated behavior is aimed at bringing us closer to a desired state. As such, our definition of behavior is broad—it can include changes in attention, perception, thoughts, and emotions, and it need not always involve overt actions. For example, imagine that we’re running late for work when our young child shatters a jar of jam all over the kitchen floor. Our initial response may be anger, but that conflicts with the goal of being a calm and supportive parent. In that context, we can bring ourselves closer to this desired state through “cognitive reappraisal” of the situation. Here, we can reappraise our child’s action as an attempt to be helpful or more grown-up by getting their own breakfast jam from the refrigerator. Research shows that such motivated efforts to regulate our emotions have a very wide scope indeed (Uusberg et al., 2019). For example, a recent large-scale study of more than 20,000 participants from 87 countries found evidence that people can effectively reduce negative