Gair Rhydd 1073 - 22nd February 2016

Page 28

28 SCIENCE

science Pakinee Pooprasert

While a few patients did not respond dramatically to the placebos, most did and this poses a very promising future for the field.

Tom Morris

Obese people have more anticipated effort for walking which leads to increased judgement of distances.

Editors: Maria Mellor Lizzie Harrett @GairRhyddSci science@gairrhydd.com gairrhydd.com/science

Possible placebo cure found for Parkinson’s

P

arkinson’s is a progressive disease where the brain becomes damaged over many years, with the classical symptoms being tremor (involuntary shaking of particular parts of the body), slow movement and stiff and inflexible muscles. Currently patients with Parkinson’s are given multiple drugs to slow down the disease progression, and many studies are conducted with the aim of finding a Parkinson’s cure or treating it more effectively. In a revolutionising study, Italian neuroscientists report that the brain cells of people with Parkinson’s disease can respond to inert (placebo) drugs. This response wears off after one day, but the effects demonstrates that it might be possible to reduce the medication needed to treat this disease by interspersing real drugs with placebo injections or pills. While a few patients did not respond dramatically to the placebos, most did and this poses a very promising future for the field. In patients with Parkinson’s, the characteristic tremors and stiff muscles are due to the death of dopamine-producing brain cells. Currently, doctors alleviate these symptoms by prescribing drugs such as apomorphine, which activates dopamine receptors. Additionally, for conditions such as pain and immune disorders, tri-

als have shown that it is possible to train people to respond to placebos, and this made Benedetti and his colleagues - researchers who led the work at the University of Turin wonder whether the same applies for neurological disorders. The study involved 42 people with advanced Parkinson’s disease who were having deep brain stimulation, allowing Benedetti’s team to measure the activity of individual neurons in the thalamus, a region that is inhibited by lack of dopamine in Parkinson’s patients. During the surgery for deep brain stimulation, participants were given a saline injection which was told to be apomorphine. Interestingly, it produced no response, unless the patients had been preconditioned by having received 1-4 daily injections of the actual drug before the surgery. It was observed that neurons showed increased activity and muscle rigidity dropped in patients who did respond to the saline after the injection. This was assessed by a neurologist who was not told of the purpose of the study, to prevent any bias. Furthermore, it was found that the larger the number of previous apomorphine injections, the greater the subsequent response to the saline. This meant that if the patients had received four previous injections, there was “no difference between

drug and placebo response,” Benedetti explained. While this training is short lived, it only lasted a maximum of 24 hours, Benedetti hopes that it may be possible to lengthen the placebo ‘memory’ of patients by giving them the real drugs for longer. Why is this study revolutionary? “Because of its clear demonstration that clinical response and neurological activation are clearly linked and can be trained,” stated Christopher Goezt, a neurologist at Rush University Medical Centre in Chicago, Illinois. Furthermore, “Though the group sizes are small, the results

seem compelling,” says Tor Wager, another neuroscientist at the University of Colorado Boulder. In addition, Espay, a neuroscientist at the University of Cincinnati in Ohio who has studied placebo effects in Parkinson’s predicts that placebos could ultimately be used in clinical practice to reduce the amount and cost of medication. Despite the clear results, what remains unclear is how exactly the effects works. Goetz postulates that it may be a ‘Pavlovian response’, in which patients conditioned through experience associate injections with symptomatic relief.

Pictured: Patients were given placebo injections (Photographer: John Donges)

Distances seem further for obese people

O

verweight and obese people see distances as at least ten percent longer than skinnier people do. It’s not just that extra distance means extra effort - it really is the case. Spatially in the brain, a mile is perceived as a greater distance the heavier you are. Psychologist Dr Jessica Witt, who investigates the perception of spatial layout, has published a few papers on the subject as well as most recently giving a talk. She wrote that “a person’s ability to perform an action affects their perception of the environment.” This makes a lot of sense, think about how short a walk it seemed to the Students’ Union from Talybont when you were in first year, but now, as you’re not used to the walk, you probably wouldn’t bother going up there. I’ve heard plenty of second and third years say “I’ve got an exam/sport up in Talybont, oh it’s such a long walk,” when they no doubt did it every day just a few months before. Witt found that when throwing a ball the same distance, the target

seemed further away if the ball was heavier. For the obese participants, extra effort was applied mentally to walking the same distance as a fit person breezed through, and thus was judged as being a longer walk. The effect was seen to apply to gradients as well as a hill may appear steeper to a heavier person that it might seem to someone slimmer. These two effects were not apparent as much in those who were just overweight as those who were obese. According to the study, a person weighing 23 stone sees objects as being twice as far away as someone who weighs nine stone. To put it simply, obese people have more anticipated effort for walking which leads to increased judgement of distances. The study also found some interesting anecdotes about body image. If an obese person only perceived themselves as overweight it made no difference, they would still have the adverse effects on judgement as their obese friends who more easily admitted to their size.

Pictured: Weight affects how you percieve distance (Photographer: Marjan Lazarevski)

“ These findings seem to show that obese people live in what one reporter described as an “altered reality,” where the extra effort caused by being heavier manifests itself as a world where everything is further away.

However, the study offers no explanation of why people sigh when anxious, sad or stressed. This could perhaps be an important area for future research, promoting collaboration between neuroscientists and psychologists.

These two effects were not apparent as much in those who were just overweight as those who were obese.


Issuu converts static files into: digital portfolios, online yearbooks, online catalogs, digital photo albums and more. Sign up and create your flipbook.