Page 1

Topic #3: Aggression

Aggression: Theories of Internal Factors Lecture #1 of 3 Kevin M. Williams August, 2005


Types of aggression • • • • • • • • •

Latin (aggredi – to attack) Physical aggression Verbal aggression Passive aggression Sexual aggression Workplace aggression Bullying Child/spouse abuse Etc…


What causes aggression? • Various theories • One of the most common debates involves internal vs. external factors • i.e., nature vs. nurture • Internal factors: We are naturally aggressive; we are born with aggressive tendencies • External factors: Our environment and experiences lead us to become aggressive


Evolutionary theories of aggression • Also called ‘instinct’ theories: – suggest aggression is a part of human nature – Aggression is an instinct, perhaps an inevitable part of human behavior – We are ‘programmed’ for violence by our biological nature


Early instinct theories • Early instinct theories (e.g., Freud) suggested that aggression stems from universal, innate tendencies • Aggression = our innate death wish/instinct (thanatos) directed outwards – (as opposed to eros – love instinct)

• Hostile impulses, which increase over time, must be released periodically (in small acts) to prevent dangerous acts of violence – catharsis hypothesis


Early evolutionary theories • Early evolutionary theories (e.g., Lorenz): – aggression stems from an inherited ‘fighting instinct’ than humans share with other species

• Fighting serves to disperse populations over wide areas (e.g., conquering new territories) – thus ensuring maximum use of resources

• Humans (and other species) physically fought over potential mates – the best fighters were able to win the mate and have their genes passed on


Modern evolutionary approaches • Behaviours that help individuals procreate will become increasingly prevalent in the species’ population • Used to explain ‘young male syndrome’: – young males are more likely to be involved in violence and risk-taking than older males or females of any age – Why? Competition for reproductive success

• Males fight with each other over resources in order to attract a female and have their genes passed on


Modern evolutionary approaches: Research • Mesquida & Wiener, 1996 • Across 88 countries from the years 1980 to 1993, the more young males in a population the more severe the group violence • Why? There are fewer resources to go around, thus hindering the young males’ ability to mate • Thus, the young males are better served to band together and fight for resources – collective aggression • Manifested in uprisings, revolutions, even war • These results are correlational, not causational – alternative explanations include poverty and political instability


Critiques of modern evolutionary approaches • Even evolutionary theorists point out that instinctive tendencies likely interact with other factors (environmental, internal) to produce aggression • Most social psychologists have rejected early instinct theories because: – The causes of human aggression are multi-faceted (not just instincts) – The fighting described by evolutionary psychologists is fairly tame, whereas typical human aggression is much more severe (e.g., leading to death) – The innate tendency to kill makes less sense than the innate tendency to do temporary harm


Critiques of modern evolutionary approaches • There are many more forms of human aggression than physical: e.g., passiveaggressive, spreading rumors, destroying property, and direct verbal aggression • Wide cultural variations in aggression: innate tendencies would predict crosscultural consistency • No support for catharsis hypothesis


Other biological theories • Genetic factors may be important – twin/adoption studies support the role of genetics in aggressive behaviour

• Neurotransmitter research: low serotonin levels may cause aggression – Serotonin is responsible for several aspects of human functioning, including mood and behaviour – Experimental studies: – lowering serotonin level in animals increases aggression – increasing serotonin levels in humans decreases aggression

• Sex hormones (e.g., testosterone: Harris et al., 1996, Van Goosen et al., 1994) – Testosterone is a male sex hormone whose functions include muscle development – Has been linked to aggressive behaviour


Aggressive personality traits • Dispositional influences on aggression • Personality traits tend to possess a strong genetic basis • Informal observation/anecdotal evidence: • some people rarely lose their temper • other people seem to have 'short fuses'


Type A Personality • Glass (1977) • One of the first attempts to identify aggressive personality traits • Type A behavioural pattern includes: – (1) extremely competitive – (2) always in a hurry – (3) especially irritable


Type A Personality: Research • Research evidence: Type A = more aggressive (e.g., hostile aggression, spousal/child abuse) • When provoked, Type A’s more likely to retaliate with a (fake) electric shock – Especially true for Type A's w/ high testosterone

• Type A not related to instrumental aggression – aggression aimed at attaining goals such as resources or praise

• Type A also believed to be related to coronary heart disease


Current state of Type A personality research • Recently, Type A personality has been replaced by trait hostility • Trait hostility explains the relationship between Type A personality and many outcomes (e.g., coronary heart disease) • Other aspects of Type A (e.g., always in a hurry, competitiveness) do not predict these outcomes


Personality traits: Researching Prisoners • Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD) – A clinical diagnosis, based strictly on behaviours – Includes criminal behaviour, recklessness, irritability

• Psychopathy – Studied predominantly in prison populations – Lack of empathy and guilt; manipulative; impusive; antisocial tendencies – Strong predictor of criminal behaviour and reoffending – Includes more emphasis on personality traits than APD (APD emphasizes behaviours)


The Dark Triad of Personality • Aversive traits found outside of prison samples • Narcissism: grandiosity, self-enhancement, self-centered • Machiavellianism: manipulative, cynical • Psychopathy: less extreme than the version found in prison samples • Although all three predict antisocial behaviour, psychopathy produces the strongest and most consistent links


Benefit of personality theories • Personality traits tend to be stable across time and situations • Thus, personality may allow for generalized predictions • E.g., people who are aggressive in the workplace will likely also be aggressive at home (spousal abuse), etc. • Would child bullies grow up to become aggressive adults?


Aggression: The role of arousal • We have already seen that many internal and external factors have been linked to aggression • Other theories involve arousal: – Any physiological excitement – e.g., rapid heartbeat, ‘adrenaline rush’, ‘butterflies in the stomach’

• But how does arousal influence aggressive behaviour?


Excitation transfer theory • Heightened arousal from an experience can lead to subsequent aggression in an unrelated situation • Various experiences may cause heightened arousal: – e.g., competitive games, vigorous exercise, amusement park rides, listening to music, scary movies – Any activity that might ‘get your heart racing’

• We may then be more likely to react to subsequent situations with aggression – e.g., if you are highly aroused, a situation that may normally only make you mildly annoyed would instead cause you to be extremely angry


Excitation transfer theory • Aggression is most likely to occur when: • persons are unaware of the presence of residual arousal (research has demonstrated that small levels of arousal tend to go unnoticed) • you notice the arousal but mistakenly attribute it to the new situation

• The nature of the post-arousal situation is highly important: • Recall we may also interpret arousal as sexual attraction (Capilano bridge study)


Sexual arousal and aggression • Sexual arousal may be used to decrease existing levels of aggression • Why? Zillmann's (1984) two-component model: • Exposure to erotic stimuli produces two effects: – (1) increases arousal – (2) influences current affective states (i.e., negative/positive moods)

• Mild erotic stimuli = weak arousal, high positive affect – reduces aggression

• Explicit stimuli = strong arousal, high negative affect (i.e., unpleasant, repulsive)


Sexual arousal and aggression • Relationship between sexual arousal and aggression is curvilinear : High

Aggression Levels

Low None

Mild

Sexual Arousal

High


Sexual arousal and aggression: Crimes of passion? • Does intense sexual arousal play a role in crimes of passion? – No direct evidence

• However, sexual jealousy, which evokes powerful feelings of anger and aggression, does appear to be related to crimes of passion • Possible that sexual jealousy may combine with high arousal to predict crimes of passion (but again, no direct evidence)


Developmental theories • E.g., moral development – Defines our understanding of right and wrong – Low moral development = think in terms of personal interest, fair exchange – High moral development = think in terms of majority interest, abstract ideals

• Research findings: Delinquency is associated with low moral development – Why? At higher levels of moral development, criminal behaviour is considered unacceptable


Summary: Internal factors and aggression • Evolutionary drives – Freud, the fighting instinct

• Biological factors – Serotonin, testosterone

• Personality traits – Type A/hostility, psychopathy, disagreeableness

• Arousal – Excitation transfer theory, sexual arousal

• Developmental theories – Moral development

• Next lecture: External factors and aggression


Topic #3: Aggression

Aggression: Theories of External Factors Lecture #2 of 3 Kevin M. Williams August, 2005


External influences on aggression • So far, we have examined internal influences on aggression: – Genetics, hormones, evolutionary drives, personality, arousal, moral development – i.e., nature

• Alternatively, researchers have been interested in external influences on aggression – i.e., nurture

• e.g., what roles do the environment and our social experiences play in predicting aggressive behaviour?


Drive theories: The role of frustration • Humans have an internal motive or drive to harm others, which normally lays dormant • When exposed to certain external conditions, this drive is aroused and the result is aggression • External conditions may include loss of face or frustration External Aggressive Aggressive condition Drive Behaviour

• Summarized by the frustration-aggression hypothesis: – (1) frustration always leads to some form of aggression, and – (2) aggression always stems from frustration (i.e., there


Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Research • However, researchers subsequently determined that the frustration-aggression hypothesis is only partially correct • Frustration leads to aggression because frustration is an aversive, unpleasant feeling • However, frustration is not the only aversive emotion: – others include anger, nervousness, impatience, irritability, etc.

• Other research evidence: frustration is more likely to produce aggression when it is either unexpected or illegitimate • Why? Likely because these types of frustration are more aversive than when it is expected or legitimate


Social learning theory • Bandura (1973) • Aggression is learned either through direct experience or by observing others • Evidenced in how different cultures express aggression (e.g., martial arts vs. firearms) • Through these channels, individuals also learn: – (1) appropriate targets for aggression – (2) what actions by others require or justify aggression, and – (3) the situations or contexts in which aggression is appropriate

• This model has been particularly helpful in describing spousal abuse – i.e., some husbands abuse their wives because they have seen their fathers abuse their mothers


Media violence


Media violence • Most of us observe much more violence through media than we are ever to encounter first-hand in real life • This point is true even if you remove real violence we observe through the media (e.g., on the news, etc.) • What affect (if any) does this exposure have on our own aggressive behaviour?


Media Violence: Research

• Exposure to media violence may indeed increase levels of aggression • E.g., short-term laboratory experiments: – individuals exposed to aggressive media, then given an opportunity to aggress against another individual (usually a confederate) – Compared to individuals who are exposed to non-violent media, these individuals subsequently aggress more (however, the differences are often quite small)

• Similar results for longitudinal studies, and the results are consistent across countries • It is important to note that the majority of these studies are correlational in nature – i.e., does media influence aggression, or do existing aggressive tendencies influence media preferences?


Media violence: Other evidence • Individuals may learn new ways of aggressing from watching TV and movies; ways they would not have imagined before (e.g., "copycat crimes") • The Dallas Morning News , July 1, 1999: – “7 year old kid kills his 3 year old brother with a wrestling move.”

• Los Angeles Times , January 8, 1999: – “Alleged Rapists Blame ‘Springer’.”

• Los Angeles Times , January 15, 1998: – “Son, Nephew Inspired by 'Scream' Movies kill woman, Police Say.”


Media violence: The role of individual differences • Reciprocal determinism theory: (Bandura, 1977) • Certain individuals are attracted to certain types of media (e.g., violent) • These individuals are also affected by this media differently than people who are not attracted to it


Research evidence • Antisocial personalities (e.g., psychopathy) are more attracted to violent media of various kinds (e.g., music, movies, video games sports) • Engaging in violent sports predicts aggressive behaviour only for psychopaths • No aggression among non-psychopaths who engage in violent sports, or psychopaths who do not engage in violent sports


Hierarchical-Mediational Confluence (HMC) Model • Malamuth (1986) • Only specific combinations of risk factors predict sexual aggression • Risk factors include pornography use, psychopathy, pro-rape attitudes • e.g., pornography use associated with sexual aggression only for psychopaths • Note: Likely because only psychopaths are using violent pornography


Cultural differences in media effects • The effects of media violence differ as a function of personality (e.g., psychopathy), but also consider culture: • Canadians and Americans are exposed to virtually the same amount of media violence, yet there is much more violence in the U.S.


Explaining the media violenceaggression link • Desensitization: – With repeated exposure over time, individuals become hardened to the suffering of others; emotional reactions become lower

• Priming of hostile thoughts: – violent media causes violence to become 'hardwired' as a reflex reaction to provocations, for example


Workplace aggression

• Presumably another example of social forces causing aggression (e.g., being fired) • Acts of workplace aggression tend to steal the headlines, but: • 1) most workplace aggression is conducted by individuals from outside the workplace (e.g., thieves) • 2) it is very rare to be a victim of workplace violence

– odds roughly 1 in 450,000 – more likely in high-risk jobs (e.g., police)

• Nonetheless, a problem worthy of scientific study


Types of workplace aggression • Most workplace aggression falls into one of three categories (Baron et al., 1999): • 1) Expressions of hostility – behaviors that are primarily verbal or symbolic in nature – e.g., gossiping, belittling others' opinions, talking behind their backs

• 2) Obstructionism – behaviors designed to obstruct or impede others' performance – e.g., failure to return phone calls or respond to memos, failure to transmit needed information, interference with others' important activities

• 3) Overt aggression – behaviors that have typically been included under the heading "workplace violence" – e.g., physical assault, theft or destruction of property, threats of physical violence


Workplace aggression: Frequency and causes • Expressions of hostility and obstructionism (i.e., covert aggression) occur much more frequently than overt aggression • Common causes of workplace aggression: – Job loss (e.g., downsizing, layoffs, increased use of part-time and temporary employees) – Perceived unfairness (e.g., unjustly fired, passed over for promotion, etc.) • more serious than just feeling unappreciated

• Importance of individual differences: – Not everyone who experiences job loss or unfairness reacts with aggression


The role of cognitions in aggression • Aggression is often the result of provocation (a social factor) – E.g., Chermack et al. (1997): as the level of provocation increased, participants gave stronger retaliation (electric shocks) – In other words, individuals want to 'pay back' any aggression they are exposed to with an equal level of aggression, or in some cases, even more aggression

• However, other peoples’ intentions are not always clear: – there is a subjective element to provocation


Cognition and provocation • Hostile attribution bias (HAB) • Tendency to perceive aggression in others' intentions even when there is none present (i.e., ambiguous or neutral situations) • For example, accidentally bumping into someone in a crowded area, or even simple eye contact – An HAB individual will be more likely to perceive these acts as hostile

• In male juvenile offenders, HAB is related to number of interpersonally violent crimes (Dodge et al., 1990)


Personality and provocation • Narcissists react to insults with extreme aggression (Bushman & Baumeister, 1998) • Why? 'Unstable high self-esteem‘ – In theory, narcissists have 'nagging doubts' about their superiority, therefore react with intense anger to anyone who threatens their ego

• Psychopaths are easily provoked, and react with vengeance and violence • Psychopaths hold hostile biases, which partially explains their delinquency (Nathanson et al., 2004)


Culture and provocation: Cultures of honor

• E.g., Southern U.S. • Overly protective of property and reputation • In these cultures, individuals are:

– More likely to respond to threats with anger and violence – More accepting of violence (and even murder) as a problem-solving strategy – More accepting of violent people/murderers – Also show increases in testosterone when provoked (recall biological theories of aggression)


An integrative theory: General Affective Aggression Model • Anderson, 1996 (GAAM) • Integrates the influence of internal and external forces in predicting aggression • Aggression is triggered/elicited by various input variables – either aspects of the situation or pre-existing individual differences • Once triggered, there are various routes to aggression


GAAM input variables • Situational factors can include – – – – –

Frustration Provocation Aggressive models (i.e., other people acting aggressively) Cues associate with aggression (e.g., guns, weapons) Virtually anything that causes discomfort (an annoying sound, hot and humid weather)

• Individual differences include: – Personality (irritability, hostility, Dark Triad, disagreeableness) – Attitudes/beliefs about violence (i.e., that it is acceptable/appropriate) – Values about violence (i.e., that it is a ‘good’ thing – that there are benefits) – ‘Aggression skills’ (knowing how to fight, use weapons, etc.)


GAAM routes to aggression • These two types of input variables can then lead to aggression through three routes: – 1) arousal (increase in physiological arousal/excitement) – 2) affect (hostile feelings – e.g., anger; or outward manifestations of feelings such as angry facial expressions) – 3) cognitions (having hostile thoughts or remembering hostile memories)


GAAM model of aggression Individual differences

Arousal

Situational factors

Affect

Aggression

Cognitions


GAAM: Exacerbating and restricting factors

• In the GAAM model, aggression is a more likely end result if the individual has hostile appraisals of the situation – (i.e., hostile cognitive biases)

• Aggression is less likely if restraining factors are present – e.g., police, strong social norms, an aggressive target, etc.


GAAM research • Research has generally supported the GAAM model of aggression: • Playing violent video games (situational input) increases hostile cognitive biases (i.e., attributing hostility to an ambiguous situation)


Summary: External factors and aggression • External factors: – Frustration, social learning, media violence, provocation, extreme occupational hardships

• Currently, many researchers agree that internal and external factors are important in predicting aggression • Many internal and external factors interact: – – – – –

Media violence and personality Provocation and personality Provocation and cognitive biases Provocation and culture General affective aggression model (GAAM)

• Next lecture: sex/cultural differences, studying aggression, and preventing aggression


Topic #3: Aggression

Aggression: Culture and sex differences, studying aggression, and prevention Lecture #3 of 3 Kevin M. Williams August, 2005


Sex differences in aggression • Widespread belief that males are more aggressive than females • Some research support for this belief: – Males report higher rates of aggressive behaviors than females

• But: males are also more likely to be the victims of aggression – Likely due to males aggressing against other males

• This difference persists throughout the lifespan – e.g., in people as high in age as 70 or 80

• Overall, gender differences tend to be minor, though there are some complexities: – Depends on situation and type of aggression


Sex and situation interactions • Sex differences become more apparent when examining provocations • Compared to females, males much more likely to aggress in situations when they are not provoked • There is virtually no gender difference when provocation is involved


Sex differences: Type of aggression • Males higher in direct aggression – actions aimed directly at the target and which clearly stem from the aggressor – e.g., shouting, hitting, shoving, throwing something, insulting, etc.

• Females higher in indirect aggression – actions that allow the aggressor to conceal their identity from the target – e.g., spreading rumors, talking behind the person's back, etc.

• These differences exist across the lifespan and across countries


Cultural differences • Most differences occur in subcultures within larger cultures • Examples: cultures of honor, gang subcultures, prisons • Why? Differences in: – cultural values (e.g., violence is valued/accepted more in certain cultures than others) – child-rearing habits (e.g., children taught from an early age that violence is acceptable/unacceptable) – socio-economic status (violence is justified as a means of survival)


Cultural differences: Comparisons across countries 2.6 2.1 2 2 1.4 1.2 1.2 1.1 0.2

la N S nd et w he itz & C rla er W Fra an al n nd lan ad e c d s a s e

Scotland Canada

Northern Ireland England & Wales

En g

Switzerland Sweden

Note: Columbia = 70.9

7.5

0

2

4

1996 Homicide Rates per 100,000

6

8


Studying aggression • How do we measure aggression? • Ethical concerns: – Making the research participant angry enough to aggress – Exposing participants to graphically violent media – Aggressing against other participants or innocent bystanders? – Long-term effects?

• Objectivity: – What are the ‘units’ of aggression? – Number of punches? Volume of voice?


Aggression: Research methods • There are various methods, each with advantages and disadvantages • Experimental methods: – e.g., participant is allowed to aggress against a confederate after false negative feedback – Participant administers electroshock or noise blast

• Pros: can be measured objectively • Cons: not very realistic; ethics


Aggression: Research methods • Pencil-and-paper methods: – Self-reports: Participants write down the number of times they have committed various aggressive acts – Peer-reports: Someone who knows the research participant reports their aggression

• Pros: Quick, easy, exhaustive • Cons: Reporting biases (memory, impression management, fear of punishment)


Aggression: Research methods • Implicit measures: – Hostile cognitive biases – read a story with the potential for aggression, write what might happen next – Ambiguous pictures – rate the amount of aggression in a ‘neutral’ face – Implicit association task (IAT) – do certain images match up with aggression words?

• Pros: Non-intrusive • Cons: Projective, unverifiable


Aggression: Research methods • Naturalistic observation: – Observe participants in their natural surroundings (e.g., schoolyard) – Researchers may or may not provoke participants

• Pros: Highly realistic • Cons: Time consuming, ethics, interrater agreement


Aggression: Research methods • Forensic and clinical methods: – Criminal records – Interviews by experts

• Pros: Highly objective • Cons: Time consuming; unrealistic for non-forensic or non-clinical research


Preventing and controlling aggression Capital Punishment


Preventing and controlling aggression • Is punishment an effective deterrent? • Yes – under certain conditions • i.e., punishment must be: – prompt – must follow aggressive actions as quickly as possible – certain – the probability that it will follow aggression must be very high – strong – strong enough to be highly unpleasant to potential recipients – appropriate – perceived by recipients as justified or deserved


Learning to reduce aggression • Interventions based on social learning theory • Exposure to non-aggressive models – e.g., showing examples of: – people who do not retaliate when provoked – people who are forgiving – non-violent role models (Gandhi)

• Social skills training: – i.e., teaching people to make/refuse requests without making others angry


Reducing aggression through catharsis • An early idea (1939) • Venting anger and hostility in a relatively safe way will reduce later aggression • Two important benefits: – 1) helps to reduce emotional tension – 2) since they help to eliminate anger, venting reduces the likelihood of more dangerous forms of aggression


Catharsis research: Mixed results

• Participation in various activities that are not harmful to others (e.g., working out) can reduce emotional arousal stemming from frustration or provocation • However, these effects are temporary: – arousal reappears quickly, simply by remembering the incident that made you angry in the first place

• Using overt physical aggression also does not work, and may actually increase subsequent aggression – Examples: watching media violence, hitting a punching bag, or yelling at someone – Recall effects of violent sports and pornography for psychopaths (though direction of causality remains an issue)


Cognitive interventions • Why are interventions important? Without intervention, one consequence is displaced aggression: – the tendency to lash out against unrelated/innocent others

• Apologizing to someone who is aggressive (expressing regret and asking for forgiveness) is effective in reducing their aggression


Cognitive interventions • The idea behind cognitive interventions is that when we are very angry, our ability to think clearly is reduced – e.g., ability to consider the consequences of our actions

• When we are angry we 'lose (cognitive) control' – these activities help us to recover control


Examples of cognitive interventions • Pre-attribution: – reminding yourself that a person's annoying behavior is due to external or uncontrollable forces (i.e., 'they don't mean it')

• Preventing yourself from dwelling on real or imagined wrongs – can be accomplished via distraction (watching a movie, working on a complex puzzle or other task)


Effectiveness of interventions • Most interventions are effective • e.g., anger management through cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT): – Involves relaxation, cognitive restructuring, problem-solving, stress inoculation – Useful in reducing aggression in college students, abusive parents, adolescents (Beck & Fernandez, 1998)


Exceptions: Treatment of psychopaths • Correctional programs tend to be aimed at developing empathy, conscience, interpersonal skills • Criminal psychopaths typically do not show behavioural improvements from treatment • Why? Psychopaths do not experience personal distress, see nothing wrong with their behaviour, and do not seek treatment unless forced • In some cases, psychopaths become more adept at criminal behaviour and manipulation following treatment


Summary • Overall, gender differences in aggression tend to be minor, though there are some complexities • Most cultural differences in aggression occur in subcultures within larger cultures • There are several methods of studying aggression, each with advantages and disadvantages • Various strategies for deterring and reducing aggression exist, each with varying degrees of success

نظريات العنف  

ملف بوربوينت عن العنف ضد كبار السن

Read more
Read more
Similar to
Popular now
Just for you